Skip to content

Advertisement

  • Minireview
  • Open Access

Bilateral olfaction: two is better than one for navigation

Genome Biology20089:212

https://doi.org/10.1186/gb-2008-9-3-212

  • Published:

Abstract

Do animals require bilateral input to track odors? A recent study reveals that fruit fly larvae can localize odor sources using unilateral inputs from a single functional sensory neuron, but that an enhanced signal-to-noise ratio provided by dual inputs is helpful in more challenging environments.

Keywords

  • Antennal Lobe
  • Olfactory Organ
  • Olfactory Processing
  • Improve Tracking Performance
  • Dual Input

Biological sensory systems often make use of asymmetries in sensory inputs to extract information about the environment. The visual system, for example, exploits disparities in the two-dimensional images obtained from the left and the right eyes to extract information about depth [1]. The auditory system uses the phase and intensity differences of stereo inputs to localize sound sources [2]. Relatively little is known about the importance of bilateral inputs in olfaction. Our own noses feature twin nostrils; insects have paired antennae. What advantages do such configurations provide? A recent study by Louis and colleagues [3] examined the significance of paired inputs for odor navigation in an animal offering numerous experimental advantages, the larva of the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster.

Studying how animals carry out chemotaxis, that is, how they navigate through chemical gradients, requires careful behavioral assays conducted within well-controlled spatial distributions of chemicals. In the case of olfaction, it is a significant technical challenge to generate the stable odor gradients needed for such a study. Louis et al. [3] developed a novel and clever approach: they built a small test chamber whose ceiling, an inverted 96-well plate, suspended an ordered array of droplets of sequentially diluted odorants (Figure 1). The authors confirmed that this array generated the desired airborne odor gradient within the test chamber by means of Fourier transformed infrared spectroscopy. Equipped with this well-controlled stimulus field, the authors set about examining chemotaxis in fruit fly larvae.
Figure 1
Figure 1

Investigating chemotaxis by Drosophila larvae. (a) Louis et al. [3] generated a well-structured airborne odor concentration gradient by suspending droplets of odorant at different concentrations from the ceiling of their test chamber (yellow denotes the highest concentration; black the lowest). The arrangement of droplets generated a spatial concentration distribution that varies from one end of the chamber to the other and from the middle of the chamber (high) to the sides. (b) Both unilateral and bilateral transgenic larvae navigate odor fields by detecting local concentration gradients. By moving along the direction of the steepest intensity variation, the larvae reliably locate the source of the odor.

In the larva, the transduction of chemical stimuli into neural representations begins in two dorsally located olfactory organs that are about 100 micrometers apart. Each olfactory organ normally contains 21 sensory neurons, each expressing one or two receptor genes together with the universally coexpressed OR83b gene [4]. Earlier studies by the authors had established that knocking out the OR83b co-receptor gene removes essentially all odor-driven behavior in these larvae [4]. By randomly rescuing the co-receptor gene in either the left or the right olfactory organ in transgenic OR83b knockout preparations, the authors generated unilateral animals - perfect for answering interesting questions about bilateral chemoreception.

Is one just as good as two?

Do the larvae require a full complement of receptors to reliably locate odor sources? Surprisingly, transgenic larvae with unilateral input from a single olfactory neuron were able to locate odor sources just as well as wild-type larvae. In fact, bilateral transgenic larvae with a single functional receptor neuron in each of their olfactory organs actually showed greater odor sensitivity than wild-type larvae. This apparently odd result may point toward an odor-coding scheme in the wild type in which ensembles of sensors with a low signal-to-noise ratio are combined with inputs with a high signal-to-noise ratio. Or, alternatively, in the wild type, competition among downstream neurons driven by different receptor neurons could diminish overall sensitivity. Schemes like these may function to promote odor discrimination, another task mediated by the same circuitry.

The authors found that both transgenic and wild-type larvae navigate by constantly orienting themselves along the direction of the steepest local concentration gradient (Figure 1). The larval rate of turning was greatest in low-concentration regions and decreased as the larvae progressed towards the concentration peak. This 'direct chemotaxis' is strikingly different from the 'biased random walk' strategy used by bacteria, which change direction at random, but alter the intervals between turns to bias movement toward attractants and away from repellants [5].

Interestingly, the authors noticed a side-dependent bias in the unilateral animals. Both left- and right-sided animals have a single functional receptor neuron, yet right-sided larvae performed chemotaxis significantly better than their left-sided counterparts. In larvae (unlike in adult flies) sensory inputs from each side remain segregated throughout the peripheral olfactory pathway. Thus, the observed right-side bias suggests disparities in downstream processing. This inherent right-side bias is not unique to these larvae - lateralization of olfactory processing has also been reported in a few other invertebrate species [6]. The importance of this bias for odor processing and olfactory behavior remains unclear.

The two olfactory organs are so close together in fruit fly larvae that any odor concentration differences between them would be undetectably slight, and so it seems unlikely that bilateral concentration comparisons could provide useful cues for successful navigation. So how do these organisms locate odor sources? The most likely possibility is that the larvae use a mechanism that allows comparisons between at least two consecutive concentration measurements made over time. Thus, the results from Louis et al. [3] suggest that a form of working memory of the concentration of recent samples is required for chemotaxis by Drosophila larvae.

Navigating complex environments

Declarations

Authors’ Affiliations

(1)
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, NIH, Lincoln Drive, Bethesda, MD 20892, USA
(2)
National Institute of Standards and Technology, Gaithersburg, MD 20899, USA

References

  1. Ohzawa I, DeAngelis GC, Freeman RD: The neural coding of stereoscopic depth. NeuroReport. 1997, 8: iii-xii.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. King AJ, Schnupp JWH, Doubell TP: The shape of ears to come: dynamic coding of auditory space. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001, 5: 261-270. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01660-0.PubMedView ArticleGoogle Scholar
  3. Louis M, Huber T, Benton R, Sakmar TP, Vosshall LB: Bilateral sensory input enhances chemotaxis. Nature Neurosci. 2008, 11: 187-199. 10.1038/nn2031.PubMedView ArticleGoogle Scholar
  4. Larsson MC, Domingos AI, Jones WD, Chiappe ME, Amrein H, Vosshall LB: Or83b encodes a broadly expressed odorant receptor essential for Drosophila olfaction. Neuron. 2004, 43: 703-714. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.019.PubMedView ArticleGoogle Scholar
  5. Berg HC: Motile behavior of bacteria. Physics Today. 2000, 53: 24-29. 10.1063/1.882934.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  6. Letzkus P, Ribi W, Wood J, Zhu H, Zhang S, Srinivasan M: Lateralization of olfaction in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Curr Biol. 2006, 16: 1471-1476. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.060.PubMedView ArticleGoogle Scholar
  7. Rodrigues V: Spatial coding of olfactory information in the antennal lobe of Drosophila melanogaster. Brain Res. 1988, 453: 299-307. 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90170-9.PubMedView ArticleGoogle Scholar
  8. Raghan R, Clement JP, Bhalla US: Rats smell in stereo. Science. 2006, 311: 666-670. 10.1126/science.1122096.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  9. Porter J, Craven B, Khan RM, Chang S, Kang I, Judkewitz B, Volpe J, Settles G, Sobel N: Mechanisms for scent tracking in humans. Nat Neurosci. 2007, 10: 27-29. 10.1038/nn1819.PubMedView ArticleGoogle Scholar
  10. Keene AC, Waddell S: Drosophila olfactory memory: single genes to complex neural circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007, 8: 341-354. 10.1038/nrn2098.PubMedView ArticleGoogle Scholar

Copyright

© BioMed Central Ltd 2008

Advertisement